new notes from henriettes thesis
This commit is contained in:
parent
3880e32fdc
commit
a52dc65626
@ -26,3 +26,7 @@
|
||||
\@writefile{toc}{\contentsline {subsection}{\numberline {3.1}Notes:}{2}{subsection.3.1}}
|
||||
\@writefile{toc}{\contentsline {section}{\numberline {4}Results}{3}{section.4}}
|
||||
\@writefile{toc}{\contentsline {section}{\numberline {5}Discussion}{3}{section.5}}
|
||||
\@writefile{toc}{\contentsline {section}{\numberline {6}Possible Sources}{3}{section.6}}
|
||||
\@writefile{toc}{\contentsline {subsection}{\numberline {6.1}Henriette Walz - Thesis}{3}{subsection.6.1}}
|
||||
\@writefile{toc}{\contentsline {subsubsection}{\numberline {6.1.1}Nervous system - Signal encoding}{3}{subsubsection.6.1.1}}
|
||||
\@writefile{toc}{\contentsline {subsubsection}{\numberline {6.1.2}electrosensory system - electric fish}{5}{subsubsection.6.1.2}}
|
||||
|
@ -1,4 +1,4 @@
|
||||
This is pdfTeX, Version 3.14159265-2.6-1.40.16 (TeX Live 2015/Debian) (preloaded format=pdflatex 2018.11.12) 22 JUN 2020 14:03
|
||||
This is pdfTeX, Version 3.14159265-2.6-1.40.16 (TeX Live 2015/Debian) (preloaded format=pdflatex 2018.11.12) 5 JUL 2020 12:01
|
||||
entering extended mode
|
||||
restricted \write18 enabled.
|
||||
%&-line parsing enabled.
|
||||
@ -499,32 +499,42 @@ l.69 \newpage
|
||||
\tf@toc=\write4
|
||||
\openout4 = `Masterthesis.toc'.
|
||||
|
||||
[2]
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Empty hook `BeforeClearDocument' on input line 163.
|
||||
[3]
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Empty hook `AfterLastShipout' on input line 163.
|
||||
[2] [3]
|
||||
(/usr/share/texlive/texmf-dist/tex/latex/ucs/data/uni-32.def
|
||||
File: uni-32.def 2013/05/13 UCS: Unicode data U+2000..U+20FF
|
||||
) [4]
|
||||
(/usr/share/texlive/texmf-dist/tex/latex/ucs/data/uni-34.def
|
||||
File: uni-34.def 2013/05/13 UCS: Unicode data U+2200..U+22FF
|
||||
)
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Empty hook `BeforeClearDocument' on input line 264.
|
||||
[5]
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Empty hook `AfterLastShipout' on input line 264.
|
||||
|
||||
(./Masterthesis.aux)
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Executing hook `AtVeryEndDocument' on input line 163.
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Executing hook `AtEndAfterFileList' on input line 163.
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Executing hook `AtVeryEndDocument' on input line 264.
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Executing hook `AtEndAfterFileList' on input line 264.
|
||||
Package rerunfilecheck Info: File `Masterthesis.out' has not changed.
|
||||
(rerunfilecheck) Checksum: EAE1F86B6F3D151F56F19C2521AD77A8;282.
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Empty hook `AtVeryVeryEnd' on input line 163.
|
||||
(rerunfilecheck) Checksum: 899A2D781B0140E88221C97161A12968;589.
|
||||
Package atveryend Info: Empty hook `AtVeryVeryEnd' on input line 264.
|
||||
)
|
||||
Here is how much of TeX's memory you used:
|
||||
10049 strings out of 493029
|
||||
145585 string characters out of 6136233
|
||||
258475 words of memory out of 5000000
|
||||
13445 multiletter control sequences out of 15000+600000
|
||||
10988 strings out of 493029
|
||||
155721 string characters out of 6136233
|
||||
259481 words of memory out of 5000000
|
||||
14339 multiletter control sequences out of 15000+600000
|
||||
8867 words of font info for 32 fonts, out of 8000000 for 9000
|
||||
1141 hyphenation exceptions out of 8191
|
||||
37i,11n,38p,339b,551s stack positions out of 5000i,500n,10000p,200000b,80000s
|
||||
37i,11n,38p,339b,561s stack positions out of 5000i,500n,10000p,200000b,80000s
|
||||
</usr/share/texlive/texmf-dist/fonts/type1/public/amsfonts/cm/cmbx12.pfb></us
|
||||
r/share/texlive/texmf-dist/fonts/type1/public/amsfonts/cm/cmr12.pfb></usr/share
|
||||
/texlive/texmf-dist/fonts/type1/public/amsfonts/cm/cmr17.pfb>
|
||||
Output written on Masterthesis.pdf (4 pages, 47088 bytes).
|
||||
/texlive/texmf-dist/fonts/type1/public/amsfonts/cm/cmr17.pfb></usr/share/texliv
|
||||
e/texmf-dist/fonts/type1/public/amsfonts/cm/cmr8.pfb></usr/share/texlive/texmf-
|
||||
dist/fonts/type1/public/amsfonts/cm/cmsy10.pfb></usr/share/texlive/texmf-dist/f
|
||||
onts/type1/public/amsfonts/cm/cmsy8.pfb>
|
||||
Output written on Masterthesis.pdf (6 pages, 82308 bytes).
|
||||
PDF statistics:
|
||||
98 PDF objects out of 1000 (max. 8388607)
|
||||
88 compressed objects within 1 object stream
|
||||
35 named destinations out of 1000 (max. 500000)
|
||||
49 words of extra memory for PDF output out of 10000 (max. 10000000)
|
||||
182 PDF objects out of 1000 (max. 8388607)
|
||||
166 compressed objects within 2 object streams
|
||||
74 named destinations out of 1000 (max. 500000)
|
||||
81 words of extra memory for PDF output out of 10000 (max. 10000000)
|
||||
|
||||
|
@ -4,3 +4,7 @@
|
||||
\BOOKMARK [2][]{subsection.3.1}{Notes:}{section.3}% 4
|
||||
\BOOKMARK [1][]{section.4}{Results}{}% 5
|
||||
\BOOKMARK [1][]{section.5}{Discussion}{}% 6
|
||||
\BOOKMARK [1][]{section.6}{Possible Sources}{}% 7
|
||||
\BOOKMARK [2][]{subsection.6.1}{Henriette Walz - Thesis}{section.6}% 8
|
||||
\BOOKMARK [3][]{subsubsection.6.1.1}{Nervous system - Signal encoding}{subsection.6.1}% 9
|
||||
\BOOKMARK [3][]{subsubsection.6.1.2}{electrosensory system - electric fish}{subsection.6.1}% 10
|
||||
|
Binary file not shown.
Binary file not shown.
@ -105,7 +105,7 @@ Außerdem erkläre ich, dass die eingereichte Arbeit weder vollständig noch in
|
||||
\item sensory perception
|
||||
\begin{enumerate}
|
||||
\item receptor -> heterogenic population
|
||||
\item further analysis limited by what receptors code for
|
||||
\item further analysis limited by what receptors code for - P-Units encoding
|
||||
\item p-type neurons code AMs
|
||||
\end{enumerate}
|
||||
|
||||
@ -192,7 +192,106 @@ Außerdem erkläre ich, dass die eingereichte Arbeit weder vollständig noch in
|
||||
\item encoding is noisy (Mainen and Sejnowski 1995, Tolhurst et al 1983, Tomko and Crapper 1974 -> review Faisal et al 2008) in part because of stimulus properties but also cell properties (Ion channel stochasticity (van Rossum et al.,2003))
|
||||
\item noise can be beneficial to encoding -> “stochastic
|
||||
resonance” (weak stimuli on thresholding devices like neurons, noice allows coding of sub threshold stimuli) (Benzi et al., 1981)
|
||||
\item single neurons are anatomically and computationally independant units, the
|
||||
representation and processing of information in vertebrate nervous systems is distributed
|
||||
over groups or networks of cells (for a review, see Pouget et al., 2000)
|
||||
\item It has
|
||||
been shown that the synchrony among cells carries information on a very fine temporal
|
||||
scale in different modalities, from olfaction (Laurent, 1996) to vision (Dan et al., 1998)
|
||||
\item In the electrosensory system it was shown before that communica-
|
||||
tion signals change the synchrony of the receptor population (Benda et al., 2005, 2006)
|
||||
and that this is read out by cells in the successive stages of the electrosensory pathway
|
||||
(Marsat and Maler, 2010, 2012; Marsat et al., 2009).
|
||||
\item An advantage of rate coding in populations is that it is fast. The rate in
|
||||
single neurons has to be averaged over a time window, that is at least as long as the
|
||||
minimum interspike interval. In contrast, the population rate can follow the stimulus
|
||||
instantaneous, as it does not have to be averaged over time but can be averaged over
|
||||
cells (Knight, 1972a).
|
||||
\item In a population of neurons subject to neuronal noise, stochastic resonance occurs
|
||||
even if the stimulus is strong enough to trigger action potentials itself (supra-threshold
|
||||
stochastic resonance described by Stocks, 2000; see Fig. 1.1 B
|
||||
\item Cells of the same type and from the same population often vary in their stimulus sen-
|
||||
sitivity (Ringach et al., 2002) as well as in their baseline activity properties (Gussin et al., 2007; Hospedales et al., 2008)
|
||||
\item Heterogeneity has been shown to improve infor-
|
||||
mation coding in both situations, in the presence of noise correlations, for example in
|
||||
the visual system cells (Chelaru and Dragoi, 2008) or when correlations mainly originate
|
||||
from shared input as in the olfactory system (Padmanabhan and Urban, 2010)
|
||||
\item A prerequisite to a neural code thus is that it can be read out by other neurons (Perkel
|
||||
and Bullock, 1968).
|
||||
\item Developement and evolution shape the func-
|
||||
tioning of many physiological systems and there is evidence that they also shape the
|
||||
encoding mechanisms of nervous systems. For example, the development of frequency
|
||||
selectivity in the auditory cortex has been shown to be delayed in animals stimulated
|
||||
with white noise only (Chang and Merzenich, 2003). Also, several encoding mecha-
|
||||
nisms can be related to the selective pressure that the energetic consumption of the ner-
|
||||
vous system has exerted on its evolution (Laughlin, 2001; Niven and Laughlin, 2008).
|
||||
These finding conformed earlier theoretical predictions that had proposed that coding
|
||||
should be optimised to encode natural stimuli in an energy-efficient way (Barlow, 1972). -> importance of using natrual stimuli as the coding and nervous system could be optimised for unknown stimuli features not contained in the artificial stimuli like white noise.
|
||||
\end{enumerate}
|
||||
\subsubsection{electrosensory system - electric fish}
|
||||
|
||||
\begin{enumerate}
|
||||
\item For decades, studies examining the neurophysiological systems of weakly electric
|
||||
fish have provided insights into how natural behaviours are generated using relatively
|
||||
simple sensorimotor circuits (for recent reviews see: Chacron et al., 2011; Fortune, 2006;
|
||||
Marsat and Maler, 2012). Further, electrocommunication signals are relatively easy to
|
||||
describe, classify and simulate, facilitating quantification and experimental manipula-
|
||||
tion. Weakly electric fish are therefore an ideal system for examining how communica-
|
||||
tion signals influence sensory scenes, drive sensory system responses, and consequently
|
||||
exert effects on conspecific behaviour.
|
||||
\item The weakly electric fish use active electroreception to navigate and communicate under
|
||||
low light conditions (Zupanc et al., 2001).
|
||||
\item In active electroreception, animals produce
|
||||
an electric field using and electric organ (and this electric field is therefore called the
|
||||
electric organ discharge, EOD) and infer, from changes of the EOD, information about
|
||||
the location and identification of objects and conspecifics in their vicinity (e.g. Kelly
|
||||
et al., 2008; MacIver et al., 2001). However, perturbations result not only from objects
|
||||
and other fish, but also from self-motion and other factors. All of these together make
|
||||
up the electrosensory scene. The perturbed version of the fish’s own field on its skin
|
||||
is called the electric image (Caputi and Budelli, 2006), which is sensed via specialised
|
||||
receptors distributed over the body surface (Carr et al., 1982).
|
||||
\item In A. leptorhynchus, the
|
||||
dipole-like electric field (electric organ discharge, EOD) oscillates in a quasi-sinusoidal
|
||||
fashion at frequencies from 700 to 1100 Hz (Zakon et al., 2002) with males emitting at
|
||||
higher frequencies than females (Meyer et al., 1987).
|
||||
\item The EOD of each individual fish
|
||||
has a specific frequency (the EOD frequency, EODf) that remains stable in time (exhibit-
|
||||
ing a coefficient of variation of the interspikes intervals as low as $2 ∗ 10^{−4} $; Moortgat et al., 1998).
|
||||
\item During social encounters, wave-type fish often modulate the frequency as
|
||||
well as the amplitude of their field to communicate (Hagedorn and Heiligenberg, 1985).
|
||||
\item Communication signals in A. leptorhynchus have been clas-
|
||||
sified into two classes: (i) chirps are transient and stereotyped EODf excursions over
|
||||
tens of milliseconds (Zupanc et al., 2006), while (ii) rises are longer duration and more
|
||||
variable modulations of EODf, typically lasting for hundreds of milliseconds to sec-
|
||||
onds (Hagedorn and Heiligenberg, 1985; Tallarovic and Zakon, 2002). (OLD INFO ? RISES NOW OVER MINUTES/HOURS)
|
||||
\end{enumerate}
|
||||
|
||||
\subsubsection{P-Units encoding}
|
||||
|
||||
\begin{enumerate}
|
||||
\item In baseline conditions (stimulus only own EOD), they fire irregularly at a certain baseline rate. Action potentials occur approximately at a certain phase of the EOD cycle, they are phase-locked to the EOD, but only with a certain probability to each cycle. The baseline rate differs from cell to cell (compare the two example cells in Fig. 2.2 A and B, Gussin et al., 2007)
|
||||
\item Since tuberous receptors are distributed over the whole body and the EOD spans the
|
||||
whole surrounding, all P-units of a given animal are stimulated with a similar stimulus
|
||||
(see Kelly et al. (2008) for an exact model of the EOD). Their noise sources are, however,
|
||||
uncorrelated (Chacron et al., 2005b).
|
||||
\item In response to a step increase in EOD amplitude, P-units exhibit pronounced spike frequency
|
||||
adaptation (Benda et al., 2005; Chacron et al., 2001b; Nelson et al., 1997; Xu et al., 1996).
|
||||
\end{enumerate}
|
||||
|
||||
\subsubsection{Chapter 4 - other models}
|
||||
\begin{enumerate}
|
||||
\itemKashimori et al.
|
||||
(1996) built a conductance-based model of the whole electroreceptor unit and were able to qualitatively reproduce the behaviour of different types of tuberous units.
|
||||
\item Nelson
|
||||
et al. (1997) constrained a stochastically spiking model by linear filters of the previously determined P-unit frequency tuning.
|
||||
\item Kreiman et al. (2000) used the same frequency
|
||||
filters to stimulate a noisy perfect integrate-and-fire neuron with which they investi-
|
||||
gated the variability of cell responses to random amplitude modulations (RAMs).
|
||||
\item To reproduce the probabilistic phase-locked firing and the correlations of the ISIs, Chacron
|
||||
et al. (2000) used a noisy leaky integrate-and-fire model with refractoriness as well as a
|
||||
dynamical threshold.
|
||||
\item Benda et al. (2005) used a firing rate model with a negative adap-
|
||||
tation current to reproduce the high-pass behaviour of P-units.
|
||||
\end{enumerate}
|
||||
|
||||
\end{document}
|
@ -5,3 +5,7 @@
|
||||
\contentsline {subsection}{\numberline {3.1}Notes:}{2}{subsection.3.1}
|
||||
\contentsline {section}{\numberline {4}Results}{3}{section.4}
|
||||
\contentsline {section}{\numberline {5}Discussion}{3}{section.5}
|
||||
\contentsline {section}{\numberline {6}Possible Sources}{3}{section.6}
|
||||
\contentsline {subsection}{\numberline {6.1}Henriette Walz - Thesis}{3}{subsection.6.1}
|
||||
\contentsline {subsubsection}{\numberline {6.1.1}Nervous system - Signal encoding}{3}{subsubsection.6.1.1}
|
||||
\contentsline {subsubsection}{\numberline {6.1.2}electrosensory system - electric fish}{5}{subsubsection.6.1.2}
|
||||
|
Loading…
Reference in New Issue
Block a user